Monday, 19 November 2018

Tests that Fail


The US and Singaporean education systems differ in many important respects, so it’s interesting to see similar concerns being voiced in each one.   Singapore has recently begun to move away from so much standardized testing and there are the beginnings of skepticism from those who have until now been pushing the ‘Big Standardized Test’ agenda in the US (which largely started with the No Child Left Behind programme in 2001). 

In each case, the use of data to drive accountability and measure success is not in question.  The question is what data is the right data to measure? and it should be a familiar one to anyone in social policy.  The problem is that we cannot wait twenty years to measure the success of an educational programme, so we look to shorter-term measures – like academic tests.  But results from academic tests can mislead, and an example from medicine is instructive.

The use of tests has been increasingly policy-driven
over the last 20 years, but often relies on limited data

We know that high cholesterol levels can increase mortality from heart disease; so reducing cholesterol levels seems to make sense.  However, medical researcher DuBroff has argued that some medicines are able to lower cholesterol levels without reducing mortality from heart disease. We should all stop and think about that for a minute – especially those of us who take statins – as it is potentially devastating claim; if DuBroff is correct, it means we don't know if what we are doing makes sense, because just looking at the reduced cholesterol levels would be to mistake the means (reduced cholesterol) for the ends (improved mortality rates).  Now I am no expert in medicine, but the nature of the issue is at least clear.  It’s less clear in education - often because it is buried in articles that talk opaquely about ‘the reliability of near-term indicators as proxies for later life outcomes’.  In medicine it’s literally a matter of life and death; in education it’s a matter of life and life-chances.

The point is, however, the same in principle – that we are in danger of confusing the means with the ends; and while we can in general associate success in school with later success in life, it is the latter that is the goal, not the former.  School should not be just about getting better at school, any more than medicine should be just about reducing cholesterol.   Medicine and School are both about much more than just themselves.  In education, professional have long known anecdotally that there is a difference between teachers who make a difference and those who help students nail the tests, and some recent studies are now backing this up and getting some interest at policy level in various places.  Hitt et al have shown that the teachers who produce improvements in student behaviour and non-cognitive skills are not particularly likely to be the same teachers who improve test scores.  Schanzenbach and Bauer have, furthermore, shown that some of the preschool programs that produced the most impressive improvements in later-life outcomes did so without producing lasting gains on test scores.   Let me repeat; there is a difference between teachers who make a difference and those who help students nail the tests. 

Perhaps, though, the most compelling reason to worry about using tests to measure educational effectiveness – at least in the US – is that after almost two decades of very significant testing, the the evidence that it works is just not there.  As Paul Greene, writing in Forbes asks “Where are the waves of students now arriving on college campuses super-prepared? Where are the businesses proclaiming that today's grads are the most awesome in history? Where is the increase in citizens with great-paying jobs? Where are any visible signs that the test-based accountability system has worked?”

What’s really interesting is that these ideas are being presented by the hitherto-supporters of big testing, such as the Brookings Institute and conservative American Enterprise Institute.  It’s a welcome change that the complexity of education is finally being recognized – but that this simply echoes what classroom teachers have been saying for about 20 years is less a cause for celebration than a cause to wonder why the professionals were ignored in the first place.  The impoverished experiences of millions of children, and the wasted billions of dollars are little short of tragedy.  Greene's comment should be read by all educational policymakers: After 20 years, folks are starting to figure out that teachers were actually correct.


References

DuBroff R, de Lorgeril M. Cholesterol confusion and statin controversy. World
Journal of Cardiology. 2015;7(7):404-409. doi:10.4330/wjc.v7.i7.404.

Greene P (2018) Is The Big Standardized Test A Big Standardized Flop?  Forbes Online. September 20, 2018

McShane, M., Wolf, P. and Hitt, C. (2018) Do impacts on test scores even matter? Lessons from long-run outcomes in school choice research. American Enterprise Institute,

Schanzenbach, D. and Bauer, L. (2016), The Long-Term Impact of the Head Start Program. Brookings Institution, August 2016. 




1 comment:

  1. This article also links to how the measuring of educational attainment is shaping what schools are looking like. https://newint.org/features/2016/04/01/edu-businesses-impact/
    (what data we collect and how it is used as...)....objective evaluation data, since it depends on your view of what education is and what it is for. If education is a commodity, deliverable with the aid of scripted lessons and the rest of the paraphernalia of an ‘Academy-in-a-box’ and measurable through a few simple standardized tests, then there’s probably nothing to worry about. Pearson will indeed be always learning – and always earning.

    If, on the other hand, education that is worth the name is not reducible to these processes and these products

    ReplyDelete